2021年6月大学英语四级CET4试卷及答案解析-第一套
2021-06-15
来源:长沙新东方
作者:长晓终
Part III Reading Comprehension (40 minutes)
Section A
Directions: In this section, there is a passage with ten blanks. You are required to select one word for each blank from a list of choices given in a word bank following the passage. Read the passage through carefully before making your choices. Each choice in the bank is identified by a letter. Please mark the corresponding letter for each item on Answer Sheet 2 with a single line through the centre. You may not use any of the words in the bank more than once.
Most animals seek shade when temperatures in the Sahara soar to 120 degrees Fahrenheit. Saharan silver ants seek lunch, skittering from underground lairs into the sun’s brutal rays to scavenge animal carcasses. In 2015 they were joined by scientists from two Belgian universities, who spent a sweltering month tracking the ants and digging out their nests. The goal: to discover how the species adapted to the kind of heat that can melt shoes.
Back in Belgium, the scientists looked at the ants under an electron microscope and found that their dense, triangular hair reflects light like a prism, giving them a metallic glint and shielding them from the sun’s heat. When Ph.D. student Quentin Willot shaved an ant with a tiny scalpel and put it under a heat lamp, its temperature jumped. He says the ants’ method of staying cool is unique among animals. Could this reflective type of hair protect people? Willot says companies are interested in reproducing it.
A) adaptingE) extremeI) M) thick
B) F) huntJ) removedN) tiny
C) crawlingG) literallyK) speciesO) unique
D) H)L)
Section B
Directions: In this section, you are going to read a passage with ten statements attached to it. Each statement contains information given in one of the paragraphs. Identify the paragraph from which the information is derived. You may choose a paragraph more than once. Each paragraph is marked with a letter. Answer the questions by marking the corresponding letter on Answer Sheet 2.
What happens when a language has no words for numbers?
Numbers do not exist in all cultures. There are numberless hunter-gatherers embedded deep in Amazonia, living along branches of the world’s largest river tree. Instead of using words for precise quantities, these people rely exclusively on terms analogous to “a few” or “some.”
In contrast, our own lives are governed by numbers. As you read this, you are likely aware of what time it is, how old you are, your checking account balance, your weight and so on. The exact (and exacting) numbers we think with impact everything from our schedules to our self-esteem.
But, in a historical sense, numerically fixated people like us are the unusual ones. For the bulk of our species’ approximately 200,000-year lifespan, we had no means of precisely representing quantities. What’s more, the 7,000 or so languages that exist today vary dramatically in how they utilize numbers.
Speakers of anumeric, or numberless, languages offer a window into how the invention of numbers reshaped the human experience. In a new book, I explored the ways in which humans invented numbers, and how numbers subsequently played a critical role in other milestones, from the advent of agriculture to the genesis of writing.
Cultures without numbers, or with only one or two precise numbers, include the Munduruku and Pirahã in Amazonia. Researchers have also studied some adults in Nicaragua who were never taught number words.
Without numbers, healthy human adults struggle to precisely differentiate and recall quantities as low as four. In an experiment, a researcher will place nuts into a can one at a time, then remove them one by one. The person watching is asked to signal when all the nuts have been removed. Responses suggest that anumeric people have some trouble keeping track of how many nuts remain in the can, even if there are only four or five in total.
This and many other experiments have converged upon a simple conclusion: When people do not have number words, they struggle to make quantitative distinctions that probably seem natural to someone like you or me. While only a small portion of the world’s languages are anumeric or nearly anumeric, they demonstrate that number words are not a human universal.
It is worth stressing that these anumeric people are cognitively normal, well-adapted to the environs they have dominated for centuries. As the child of missionaries, I spent some of my youth living with anumeric indigenous people, the aforementioned Pirahã who live along the sinuous banks of the black Maici River. Like other outsiders, I was continually impressed by their superior understanding of the riverine ecology we shared.
Yet numberless people struggle with tasks that require precise discrimination between quantities. Perhaps this should be unsurprising. After all, without counting, how can someone tell whether there are, say, seven or eight coconuts in a tree? Such seemingly straightforward distinctions become blurry through numberless eyes.
This conclusion is echoed by work with anumeric children in industrialized societies.
Prior to being spoon-fed number words, children can only approximately discriminate quantities beyond three. We must be handed the cognitive tools of numbers before we can consistently and easily recognize higher quantities.
In fact, acquiring the exact meaning of number words is a painstaking process that takes children years. Initially, kids learn numbers much like they learn letters. They recognize that numbers are organized sequentially, but have little awareness of what each individual number means. With time, they start to understand that a given number represents a quantity greater by one than the preceding number. This "successor principle" is part of the foundation of our numerical cognition, but requires extensive practice to understand.
None of us, then, is really a “numbers person.” We are not predisposed to handle quantitative distinctions adroitly. In the absence of the cultural traditions that infuse our lives with numbers from infancy, we would all struggle with even basic quantitative distinctions.
Number words and written numerals transform our quantitative reasoning as they are coaxed into our cognitive experience by our parents, peers and school teachers. The process seems so normal that we sometimes think of it as a natural part of growing up, but it is not. Human brains come equipped with certain quantitative instincts that are refined with age, but these instincts are very limited. For instance, even at birth we are capable of distinguishing between two markedly different quantities — for instance, eight from 16 things.
But we are not the only species capable of such abstractions. Compared to chimps and other primates, our numerical instincts are not as remarkable as many presume. We even share some basic instinctual quantitative reasoning with distant nonmammalian relatives like birds. Indeed, work with some other species, including parrots, suggests they too can refine their quantitative thought if they are introduced to the cognitive power tools we call numbers.
So, how did we ever invent "unnatural" numbers in the first place?
The answer is, literally, at your fingertips. The bulk of the world’s languages use base-10, base-20 or base-5 number systems. That is, these smaller numbers are the basis of larger numbers. English is a base-10 or decimal language, as evidenced by words like 14 (“four” + “10”) and 31 (“three” x “10” + “one”).
We speak a decimal language because an ancestral tongue, proto-Indo-European, was decimally based. Proto-Indo-European was decimally oriented because, as in so many cultures, our linguistic ancestors’ hands served as the gateway to realizations like “five fingers on this hand is the same as five fingers on that hand.” Such transient thoughts were manifested into words and passed down across generations. This is why the word “five” in many languages is derived from the word for “hand.”
Most number systems, then, are the by-product of two key factors: the human capacity for language and our propensity for focusing on our hands and fingers. This manual fixation — an indirect by-product of walking upright on two legs — has helped yield numbers in most cultures, but not all.
Cultures without numbers also offer insight into the cognitive influence of particular numeric traditions. Consider what time it is. Your day is ruled by minutes and seconds, but these entities are not real in any physical sense and are nonexistent to numberless people. Minutes and seconds are the verbal and written vestiges of an uncommon base-60 number system used in Mesopotamia millennia ago. They reside in our minds, numerical artifacts that not all humans inherit conceptually.
Research on the language of numbers shows, more and more, that one of our species’ key characteristics is tremendous linguistic and cognitive diversity. While there are undoubtedly cognitive commonalities across all human populations, our radically varied cultures foster profoundly different cognitive experiences. If we are to truly understand how much our cognitive lives differ cross-culturally, we must continually sound the depths of our species’ linguistic diversity.
36. [E] It is worth stressing that these anumeric people are cognitively (在认知方面)normal, well-adapted to the surroundings they have dominated for centuries.
37. [H] Compared with other mammals, our numerical instincts are not as remarkable as many assume.
38. [E] It is worth stressing that these anumeric people are cognitively(在认知方面)normal, well-adapted to the surroundings they have dominated for centuries.
39. [B] But, in a historical sense, number-conscious people like us are the unusual ones.
40. [K] Research on the language of numbers shows, more and more, that one of our species’ key characteristics is tremendous linguistic(语言的) and cognitive diversity.
41. [D] This and many other experiments have led to a simple conclusion: When people do not have number words, they struggle to make quantitative distinctions that probably seem natural to someone like you or me.
42. [G] None of us, then, is really a “numbers person.” We are not born to handle quantitative distinctions skillfully.
43. [A] Numbers do not exist in all cultures.
44. [I] So, how did we ever invent “unnatural” numbers in the first place? The answer is, literally, at your fingertips.
45. [F] This conclusion is echoed by work with anumeric children in industrialized societies.
Section C
Directions: There are 2 passages in this section. Each passage is followed by some questions or unfinished statements. For each of them there are four choices marked A), B), C) and D). You should decide on the best choice and mark the corresponding letter on Answer Sheet 2 with a single line through the centre.
初读文章时题干、选项、写作大纲、关键词这些是必读的,
08/07长沙新东方考研英语老师分享五个经验,合理规划时间、“记”单词
08/07长沙新东方考研老师整理了考研英语复习的四个方法技巧要点,第一
08/07长沙新东方考研英语老师整理了考研英语攻克完型的13大技巧,帮
08/07何复习完形填空?什么时候开始准备?完形填空选项答案有没有什么
08/07考研政治毛中特部分复习有些知识点很重要,要重点看。其中中华民
07/19考研政治毛中特部分复习有些知识点很重要,要重点看。其中建设社
07/19考研政治毛中特部分复习有些知识点很重要,要重点看。其中我国的
07/19考研政治毛中特部分复习有些知识点很重要,要重点看。其中社会主
07/19“四个全面”战略布局是新形势下推进党和国家事业发展的科学纲领
07/19考研数学复习刷题,看书固然重要,但是最重要是在这过程中要总结
07/18考研数学复习刷题,看书固然重要,但是最重要是在这过程中要总结
07/18考研数学复习刷题,看书固然重要,但是最重要是在这过程中要总结
07/18暑期拉锯战,粮草要备好。何为粮草?必然是我们的复习资料。考研
07/18考研数学复习刷题,看书固然重要,但是最重要是在这过程中要总结
07/182020年全国硕士研究生招生考试初试将于2019年12月21
11/27本篇整理了一些计算机专业复习可以注意的重点内容,分享给学生进
07/18下面是长沙新东方为大家准备的2019计算机考研到底哪些复习资
07/18长沙新东方为考生总结出特别注意计算机备考特点、复习的重点和关
07/18长沙新东方为大家分享了计算机备考六部曲,希望考研的同学们能按
07/18机会永远青睐有准备的人!规划好自己的未来,明确自己的需求。为
03/04今天我们来说一说,哪些情况不可调剂。
01/12距离2021考研初试越来越近,同学们在提升自己实力的同时,不
12/21距离2022考研只剩下整整一年!为了能更从容的应对之后的挑战
12/16其实考研它是有难度等级划分的,今天小编给大家列了一个考研难度
11/18本篇整理了2018年6月英语四级翻译真题,分享给学生进行学习
07/18本篇整理了一些2018年翻译真题,希望能够为同学们提供帮助。
07/18本篇整理了2018年6月四级阅读理解题,分享给同学进行参考和
07/18本篇为学生们收集了2018年6月英语四级选词填空真题,并有新
07/18长沙新东方为同学们准备了2018年6月英语试题的阅读理解真题
07/18阅读理解在选材方面,都源于最近的英美主流报刊杂志,文章的发表
07/182018年6月六级第一套题中的作文难度与去年12月基本持平,
07/18翻译题型主要考查考生理解和表达两个层面的能力。在理解层面,要
07/18很多同学拿到选词填空的时候,第一个步骤是把每个词汇进行通读,
07/18长篇阅读的考查目的是测试学生摘要式阅读能力,即快速筛选信息、
07/18六级口试:5月23日。自本次考试起,CET将免费提供电子成绩
03/092020年9月英语六级第三套试题长沙新东方大学部老师独及答案
09/182020年9月英语六级第二套试题长沙新东方大学部老师独及答案
09/18海南是仅次于台湾的中国第二大岛,是位于中国最南端的省份。海南
06/15铁观音(Tieguanyin)是中国最受欢迎的茶之一,原产自
06/15长沙新东方为大家提供四六级查分通道,希望同学们都取得满意的成
11/03各位同学请注意啦!四六级成绩查询已经开放!不论是正坐在电脑前
02/21四六级考试成绩将于2018年8月22日发布,查询网址为中国教
08/14点击四六级成绩查询页面,然后输入准考证号和姓名,点击查询按钮
11/032020年上半年全国大学英语四、六级考试9月延考成绩于11月
11/03